🧪 Bias, Confounders, and Clinical Trial Design: A Hidden Side of Evidence
- Gamze Bulut
- Apr 5
- 3 min read

Clinical trials are often called the gold standard in medical research — and for good reason. They’re designed to test whether a treatment works, to measure harm and benefit, and to guide clinical decision-making with data, not gut feelings.
But even gold can tarnish.
Even well-designed clinical trials can produce misleading results — and sometimes, it’s not because of bad intentions or poor science. Sometimes, it's because real life is messier than our study designs. Today I want to explore a hidden side of clinical trials: the biases, blind spots, and subtle factors that can distort what we see.
These are the things that trial designers work hard to avoid — and the things that can still sneak in.
🎯 What Can Skew Trial Results (Even When We Randomize)?
Concept | How It Distorts | Example |
Selection Bias | The people who get enrolled aren’t representative of those who’ll eventually use the treatment. | If a trial excludes older adults, the results may not apply to real patients in geriatric care. |
Confounding | A hidden variable influences both the treatment and the outcome. | Coffee drinkers might show higher cancer rates not because of coffee — but because they’re more likely to smoke. |
Effect Modification | A treatment works differently in different subgroups — and lumping them together hides this. | A blood pressure drug may be highly effective in one racial group and less so in another. |
Loss to Follow-up | When people drop out unequally between groups, the results can be biased. | If more people stop the drug because of side effects, and we only analyze those who stayed, we may underestimate harm. |
Measurement/Observer Bias | Knowing who is in which group changes how outcomes are measured or reported. | A clinician might (subconsciously) rate a patient as “improved” just because they know the patient got the real drug. |
Randomization Imbalance | In small trials, randomization doesn’t always equal balance. | One group might end up with more severe disease at baseline, skewing results despite random assignment. |
Cultural or Socioeconomic Bias | Who can participate affects who the results apply to. | Language barriers, transportation issues, or work schedules can exclude underrepresented populations. |
Underpowered Studies | Too few participants means we might miss real effects (false negatives). | A small trial says “no difference,” but it simply didn’t have enough data to know. |
Publication Bias | Trials with “positive” results are more likely to be published. | Many failed or null-result studies may be left in drawers, giving a distorted view of success. |
🧩 How Researchers Try to Fix It
The good news? Clinical trial designers are acutely aware of these problems — and they’ve developed tools to minimize them:
Randomization and stratification to ensure fair comparison
Blinding to remove observer expectations
Intention-to-treat analysis to preserve group integrity
Oversampling underrepresented groups to improve equity
Global FDR correction, pre-registration, and open reporting to reduce cherry-picking
Real-world data to supplement controlled trials
These are not perfect solutions — but they’re proof that modern science is not about pretending bias doesn’t exist. It’s about designing smart enough to account for what we cannot control.
🧠 Final Thought
Clinical trials are our best tool for answering the question: “Does this work?”
But we also need to ask: “For whom? Under what conditions? And what might we be missing?”
In evidence-based medicine, the evidence is only as strong as the lens we view it through. By sharpening that lens — and being honest about its distortions — we move closer to equity, clarity, and truth.
Comments